Ocular dominance column
Encyclopedia
Ocular dominance columns are stripes of neuron
Neuron
A neuron is an electrically excitable cell that processes and transmits information by electrical and chemical signaling. Chemical signaling occurs via synapses, specialized connections with other cells. Neurons connect to each other to form networks. Neurons are the core components of the nervous...

s in the visual cortex
Visual cortex
The visual cortex of the brain is the part of the cerebral cortex responsible for processing visual information. It is located in the occipital lobe, in the back of the brain....

 of certain mammals (including humans) that respond preferentially to input from one eye or the other. The columns span multiple cortical layers, and are laid out in a striped pattern across the surface of the striate cortex. The stripes lie perpendicular to the orientation columns.

Ocular dominance columns were important in early studies of cortical plasticity, as it was found that monocular deprivation
Monocular deprivation
Monocular deprivation is an experimental technique used by neuroscientists to study central nervous system plasticity. Generally, one of an animal's eyes is sutured shut during a period of high cortical plasticity...

 causes the columns to degrade, with the non-deprived eye assuming control of more of the cortical cells.

It is believed that ocular dominance columns must be important in binocular vision. Surprisingly, however, many squirrel monkeys either lack or partially lack ocular dominance columns, which would not be expected if they are useful. This has led some to question whether they serve a purpose, or are just a byproduct of development.

Discovery

Ocular dominance columns were discovered in the 1960s by Hubel and Wiesel
Wiesel
Wiesel may refer to:* Elie Wiesel, Hungarian-American novelist, Nobel Peace Prize laureate, philosopher, humanitarian, and Holocaust survivor* Torsten Wiesel, Swedish co-recipient of Nobel Prize in Medicine...

 as part of their Nobel prize winning work on the structure of the visual cortex in cats. They have since been found in many animals, such as ferrets, macaques, and humans. Notably, they are also absent in many animals with binocular vision, such as rats.

Structure

Ocular dominance columns are stripe shaped regions that lie perpendicular to the orientation columns in V1. They are enervated by input from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) into layer 4 and have mostly reciprocal projections to many other parts of the visual cortex.

Relation to Other features of V1

The ocular dominance columns are stripe shaped and cover the primary (striate) visual cortex. If the columns corresponding to one eye were colored, a pattern similar to that shown in the accompanying figure would be visible when looking at the surface of the cortex. However, the same region of cortex could also be colored by the direction of edge that it responds to, giving the orientation columns, which are laid out in a characteristic pinwheel shape. Similarly, there are columns in the cortex that have high levels of the protein cytochrome oxidase. These are called cytochrome oxidase "blobs" because of their scattered blob-like appearance.

All three types of column are present in the human visual cortex, and macaques . In macaques, it was found that both blobs and pinwheel centers tend to lie in the center of ocular dominance columns. No particular relation between has been found between pinwheel centers and blobs.

Most early models of the columns supposed that there were discrete "modules" or "hypercolumns" tiling the cortex, consisting of a repeating unit containing a full set of orientation and ocular dominance. While such units can be constructed, the map of columns is so distorted that there is no repeating structure and no clear boundaries between modules. Additionally, practically every combination of having or not having orientation, dominance, and cytochrome oxidase columns has been observed in one species or another. Further confusing the issue, squirrel monkeys don't always express columns, and even when they do the cytochrome oxidase blobs are not in register with the ocular dominance columns.

Formation

There is no consensus yet as to how ocular dominance columns are initially developed. One possibility is that they develop through Hebbian learning triggered by spontaneous activity in the eyes of the developing fetus, or in the LGN. Another possibility is that axonal guidance cues may guide the formation, or a combination of mechanisms may be at work. It is known that ocular dominance columns develop before birth, which indicates that if an activity dependent mechanism is involved it must work based on intrinsic activity. It is known that spontaneous waves of activity in the retina occur before birth and that these waves are crucial for eye specific segregation of in puts to the LGN. Similarly, the correlated activation for the retinal waves may direct development of the ocular dominance columns. Similar spontaneous activity in the cortex may also play a role. In any case, it has been shown that disrupting the retinal waves at least alters the pattern of ocular dominance columns.

Sensitive periods

Although the ocular dominance columns are formed before birth, there is a period after birth--formerly called a "critical period" and now called a "sensitive period"--when the the ocular dominance columns may be modified by activity dependent plasticity. This plasticity is so strong that if the signals from both eyes are blocked the ocular dominance columns will completely desegregate. Similarly, if one eye is closed ("monocular deprivation") ,removed, or silenced during the critical period, the size of the columns corresponding to the removed eye shrink dramatically.

Models

Many models have been proposed to explain the development and plasticity of the ocular dominance columns. In general these models can be split into two categories, those that posit formation via chemotaxis and those that posit a Hebbian activity dependent mechanism. Generally chemotaxis models assume activity independent formation via the action of axon guidance molecules with the structures only later being refined by activity, but there are now known to be activity dependent and activity modifying guidance molecules.

Modified Hebbian learning

One major model of the formation of the stripes seen in ocular dominance columns is that they form by Hebbian competition between axon terminals.
The ocular dominance columns look like Turing patterns, which can be formed by modified Hebbian mechanisms. In a normal Hebbian model, if two neurons are connected to a neuron and fire together, they increase the strength of the synapses, moving the axon terminals closer together. The model must be modified to incorporate incoming activity that is locally excitatory and long range inhibitory, because if this is not done then the column width will only be dependent on the width of the axonal arbor, and also segregation will often fail in the presence of inter eye correlation. This basic model has since been extended to be more physiologically plausible with the addition of long term potentiation and depression, synaptic normalizationMiller K.D (1996) Synaptic economics: competition and cooperation in correlation-based synaptic competition. Neuron, 17:371-374., neurotrophin release, reuptake , and STDP.

Chemotaxis

Chemotactic models posit the existence of axon guidance molecules that direct the initial formation of the ocular dominance columns. These molecules would guide the axons as the develop based on markers specific to the axons from each eye. All chemotactic models must take into account the activity dependent effects demonstrated in later development, but they have been called for because several pieces of evidence make entirely activity dependent formation unlikely. First, It has been shown that the ocular dominance columns in squirrel monkeys have mirror symmetry across the cortex. This is very unlikely to occur by activity dependent means because it implies a correlation between the nasal retina of one eye and the temporal retina of the other, which has not been observed. Furthermore, work in achiasmatic Belgian sheepdogs has shown that columns can form between the projections form the temporal and nasal retina of the same eye, clearly suggesting a nasal-temporal labeling, rather than contralateral vs. ipsilateral, which would be much easier to explain with activity dependent mechanisms. Despite this, a molecular label that directs the formation of the ocular dominance columns has never been found.

Function

It was has long been believed that ocular dominance columns play some role in binocular vision. Another candidate function for ocular dominance columns (and for columns in general) is the minimization of connection lengths and processing time, which could be evolutionarily important. Many believe that the ocular dominance columns serve no function

Further Reading

  • Katz LC, Crowley JC. (2002). Development of cortical circuits: lessons from ocular dominance columns. Nat Rev Neurosci, 3(1):34-42.
  • Carreira-Perpinan M,Lister R, Goodhill G. 2005.A computational model for the development of multiple maps in primary visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex 15:1222–1233.
The source of this article is wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.  The text of this article is licensed under the GFDL.
 
x
OK