Kala Lagaw Ya
Encyclopedia
Kala Lagaw Ya is a language belonging to all the western and central Torres Strait Islands
Torres Strait Islands
The Torres Strait Islands are a group of at least 274 small islands which lie in Torres Strait, the waterway separating far northern continental Australia's Cape York Peninsula and the island of New Guinea but Torres Strait Island known and Recognize as Nyumaria.The islands are mostly part of...

, Queensland
Queensland
Queensland is a state of Australia, occupying the north-eastern section of the mainland continent. It is bordered by the Northern Territory, South Australia and New South Wales to the west, south-west and south respectively. To the east, Queensland is bordered by the Coral Sea and Pacific Ocean...

, Australia
Australia
Australia , officially the Commonwealth of Australia, is a country in the Southern Hemisphere comprising the mainland of the Australian continent, the island of Tasmania, and numerous smaller islands in the Indian and Pacific Oceans. It is the world's sixth-largest country by total area...

. On some islands it has now largely been replaced by Brokan (Torres Strait Creole English). It has the highest speaker population of any indigenous language within Australian territory, with between 3000 and 4000 people speaking the language (Ethnologue
Ethnologue
Ethnologue: Languages of the World is a web and print publication of SIL International , a Christian linguistic service organization, which studies lesser-known languages, to provide the speakers with Bibles in their native language and support their efforts in language development.The Ethnologue...

) as their first language.

Before colonisation in the 1870s–1880s, it was the major lingua franca of the area in both Australia and Papua, and is still widely spoken by neighbouring Papuans and by some Aboriginal people. However many second (or nth) language speakers it has is unknown. It also has a koiné form, as well as a pidginised form. The koinè is fairly prevalent on Badu [Badhu] (Kala Lagaw Ya territory) and neighbouring Moa, particularly among younger people.

Names

The language is known by several names besides Kalaw Lagaw Ya, most of which (including Kalaw Lagaw Ya), are strictly speaking names of dialects, spelling variants, dialect variants, and the like - and include translations of the English terms, Western Island Language and Central Island Language. The following list includes most of the commonest:
  • Kalaw Kawaw Ya/Kalau Kawau Ya [Western Island Language], Lagaw Ya/Lagau Ya [Place/Home/Island Language], Kala Lagaw Langgus/Kala Lagau Langgus [Western Place/Home/Island Language], Kalaw Kawaw Langgus/Kalau Kawau Langgus [Western Island Language], Kalaw Lagaw Ya [Western Place/Home/Island Language], Langus [Language], Lingo, Kawalgaw Ya/Kaywalgaw Ya/Kaiwaligau Ya [Islanders' Language], Kowrareg (Kaurareg) (strictly speaking Kaurareg ['Islander'] is the person, not the language, which is/was Kauraregau Ya [Islanders' Language]), Kulkalgau Ya [Blood-Peoples' Language (Kulka 'Blood' was an important cult figure)], Mabuiag (the name of one of the islands where it is spoken), Westen or West Torres or Western Torres Strait, Dhadhalagau Ya [Mid-Island/Central Island Language], Sentrel or Central Islands. One term used by Eastern islanders and neighbouring Papuans for Kala Lagaw Ya is Yagar Yagar, from the word yagar (yá speech, etc. + gár sympathy clitic (dear, please, etc.), often used by Western and Central Islanders in speech to show a sympathetic or nostalgic frame of mind.


In literature on the language the abbreviations KLY (Kalaw Lagaw Ya), KKY (Kalau Kawau Ya), KulY (Kulkalgau Ya) and KY (Kaiwaligau Ya) are often used as abbreviations. In English, the most neutral and inclusive term is The Western and Central Torres Strait Language.

When speaking to each other, speakers generally refer to the language as Langgus Language or use phrases such as ngalpudh mulai- (KKY ngalpadh mulai-) , e.g. ngalpudh muuli, thanamunungu tidailai! (KKY Ngalpadh muli, thanamulngu tidaile!) Speak in our language so they don't understand!. Ngalpudh/ngalpadh literally means like us. The construction X-dh mulai- speak X-like is used to refer to speaking in a language, e.g. markaidh mulai- speak [in] English, zapanisadh mulai- speak [in] Japanese, dhaudhalgadh mulai- speak [in] Papuan, mœyamadh mulai- speak [in] Meriam Mìr, thanamudh mulai- speak like them, speak [in] their language. It is otherwise common for speakers to use nominal phrases like ngalpun ya (KKY ngalpan ya) our language to refer to the language when speaking to each other.

The other languages spoken in the Torres Strait Islands are the Meriam language
Meriam language
Meriam is the language of the people of Mer , Waier and Dauar, Erub and Ugar in the...

 and Torres Strait Creole
Torres Strait Creole
Torres Strait Creole is an English-based creole language spoken on several Torres Strait Islands , Northern Cape York and South-Western Coastal Papua...

.

Geographic Distribution

Kala Lagaw Ya is spoken on all of the western and central islands of Torres Strait
Torres Strait Islands
The Torres Strait Islands are a group of at least 274 small islands which lie in Torres Strait, the waterway separating far northern continental Australia's Cape York Peninsula and the island of New Guinea but Torres Strait Island known and Recognize as Nyumaria.The islands are mostly part of...

, between Papua New Guinea
Papua New Guinea
Papua New Guinea , officially the Independent State of Papua New Guinea, is a country in Oceania, occupying the eastern half of the island of New Guinea and numerous offshore islands...

 and the Australian mainland, though on some islands it has now been replaced by Brokan (Torres Strait Creole
Torres Strait Creole
Torres Strait Creole is an English-based creole language spoken on several Torres Strait Islands , Northern Cape York and South-Western Coastal Papua...

). Before Colonisation in the 1870s-1880s, the language was the major lingua franca of the area in both Australia and Papua, and there is some folk history evidence that the language was spoken as a first language in a few villages neighbouring Torres Strait in Papua. It was also formerly spoken by the Hiamu of Daru (Dhaaru) to the north-east of Torres Strait. The main body of the Hiamu (who were originally settlers from Yama [Yam Island] in Torres Strait) moved to the Thursday Island group to escape the Kiwai colonisation of the area.

Classification and External Comparison

Kalaw Lagaw Ya is conventionally classified among the Pama–Nyungan languages. But Mitchell (1995), Capell (1956), Dixon (2002) regard it as a mixed language with an Australian core substratum
Substratum
In linguistics, a stratum or strate is a language that influences, or is influenced by another through contact. A substratum is a language which has lower power or prestige than another, while a superstratum is the language that has higher power or prestige. Both substratum and superstratum...

 and Papuan and/or Austronesian overlays. The latter two researchers even reclassify it among the Papuan languages
Papuan languages
The Papuan languages are those languages of the western Pacific which are neither Austronesian nor Australian. The term does not presuppose a genetic relationship. The concept of Papuan peoples as distinct from Melanesians was first suggested and named by Sidney Herbert Ray in 1892.-The...

. The personal pronouns, for example, are all typically Australian, most kin terms are Papuan, and significant sea and agricultural vocabulary is Austronesian.

Kalaw Lagaw Ya has only 6% cognation with its closest Australian neighbour, Urradhi, with a further 5% "common" vocabulary [loans, and so on]) - and about 25% common vocabulary with its Papuan neighbour, Meriam Mìr. Of the 279 Proto-Paman forms given in Sommer 1969:62-66, only 18.9% have definite realisations in WCL, with a further 2.5% which may be present. One word that illustrates the problems of 'may-be' relationship is kùlbai (KKY kùlba) old, which may be a metathetic realisation of CA *bulgan big; old. Thus, where vocabulary is concerned, potentially 80% of the vocabulary of the language is non-Australian, and includes Papuan and Austronesian items (Mitchell 1995:9).

The following are a few examples of different word stock in WCL.
Australian
(Common Australian)
Papuan
(Proto–East Trans-Fly)
Austronesian
(Proto–Central District)
*nya-ga look
naga+i- id.
*sipi root
sipi id.
*gamo belly
gamu body
*jana they
thana id.
*p[ae]- that. there
pi-/pe- specifically yonder
*waura south-east
wœur(a) id.
*ganyarra reptile
kœnara k.o. tree snake
*gabo cold
gabu id.
*boro-ma pig
bùrùm(a) id.
*galga spear
kœlak(a) id.
*biro side
bero rib; side of boat, hill, river, etc.
*pu[lr]i magic etc.
puy(i) (older puuri) id.


Oral tradition and cultural evidence recorded by Haddon (1935) and Laade (1968) shows that Austronesian trade and settlement in South-West Papua, Torres Strait and Cape York occurred, backed up by archaeological evidence (Bruno, McNiven, et al. 2004) and linguistic evidence - the languages have significant Austronesian vocabulary content (cf. Dutton 1972 and 1976, Verhoeve 1982), including items such as the following which are possibly or probably Austronesian.
WCL meaning MM meaning Bine
(Papua)
meaning Proto-Oceanic
Austronesian
meaning
maapu heavy (beberbeber) (heavy) mæpu id. *mapa id.
paad(a) hill paser id. podo id. *pantar id.
-gi negative,
privative
-kak id. gika,
lika
id. *ŋkeka,
*leka
id.
Wœy(i)
OKY *wœri
Venus wer star wale star *waRi sun
wœiwi mango waiwi id. wiwi id. *waiwai id.
waaku mat; sail papék id. waaku id.
(WCL loan)
*paqu id.
waaru turtle (nam) (turtle) waaru id.
(WCL loan)
*ponu id.


Some of the Austronesian content, as noted above, is clearly South-East Papuan Austronesian:
item WCL Gudang
(Australia)
Kiwai
(Papua)
Motu
(Central District, Papua)
Proto–SE Papuan Proto-Oceanic
nacre, mother-of-pearl maay(i)
(OKY maari)
maari mari mairi *mairi ?
outrigger sayim(a)
OKY sarima
charima sarima
SE Kiwai harima
darima *nsarima *saRaman
pig bùrùm(a) ---- boroma boroma *boro-ma *mporok
rope, cord wœru
KKY wœrukam(i)
uuru waro varo *waro *waro


The linguistic history of the Torres Strait area is complex, and interaction of millennia has led to many layers of relationship between the local languages, including many words that are obviously common, though whether through trade or 'genetics' is another story. Examples of such words are the following, which through direct or indirect evidence from folk history and comparative linguistics appear likely to be loans from WCL into neighbouring languages.

Some "trade" words in Torres Strait area languages.
WCL Meriam Mìr Kiwai
(Papua)
Aböb
(Papua)
Gudang
(Australia)
Urradhi
(Australia)
Anguthimri
(Australia)
Mpakwithi
(Australia)
gii
tusk, knife, tusk/knife-life formation
gir
tusk/knife-life formation
giri
tusk, knife, tusk/
knife-life formation
? ? kiri/ghiri
knife
kiri
knife
kiri
knife
sœguba
tobacco
sogob
tobacco
suguba
tobacco
? ---* tyughubha
tobacco
tyughubhu
tobacco
?
yœuth(a)
long house, hall; church
yìut
church
---- ? ? yutha
house
---- ----
mœruka
any strange four-legged animal
---- ---- ? ? murruku
horse
? marruku
horse
mœrap(i)
bamboo
marep marabo ? marrapi marrapi ? marrapi
eso
thanks
esoau ? eso ? ? ? ?
paaudh(a)
peace
paud ? piuda paaudha ? ? ?
warup(a)
drum
warup warupa (w)arapa warrupa (w)arrupa (w)arrupa (w)arrupa
thuurik(a)
cutting tool
tulik turika turika
Bine turi/turikæ
? thurriya
crowbar
thurriya
crowbar
thurriya
crowbar

  • The only Gudang word recorded in the mid-1800s by Europeans was choki, from the Malay-based English Pidgin English used by the British (and other) sailors of the time. The Malay word is variously coki or cuki.


However, any discussion of external relationships of WCL must also take into account the fact that there are tantalising resemblances between the Paman (Australian) and the Trans-Fly (Papuan) languages, that, though few, may be significant, and include forms such as those noted below, which include words that do not appear in WCL. Such resemblances can point to an deep-level relationship dating back to before the flooding of Torres Strait at the end of the last age, as well as to later contacts.
Proto-Paman
(or a specific North Cape York language)
meaning Proto-Trans-Fly meaning WCL meaning
*kaalu ear *kuru hear kaura;
kùrusai- (compounds only)
ear
*ŋaa(na) who *ŋana id. ngaa who; also nga- found only in the compound ngalaga where (lág(a) place)
*mini good *mi:nji id. miina real, true, very
anha
Urradhi, Gudang
breath *ŋana id. ngœna id.
wintamwintama
Urradhi
star *mpintom id. thithuy(i)
OKY thithuri
id.
*nyupun one *[]pan id. wœrapùn(i)
ùrapùn
id.
*pama man, person *pama id. --- ---

Pronouns - linguistic and historical comparison

A comparison of the WCL, MM, Kiwai and Urradhi personal pronouns show similarities and differences in typology. In comparison to Urradhi, WCL has an archaic typology - or, rather, Urradhi has innovated. Kiwai, in comparison to the East Trans Fly languages, is aberrant in not having 1-2 pronouns, while MM is aberrant to itself in not having a dual and trial/paucal set of pronouns which correspond to its verb system. The WCL system in itself is essentially Australian:
WCL (KKY dialect)
number 1 1-2 2 3
singular ngai ---- ngi nui masculine
na feminine
dual ngalbe ngœba ngipel palai
plural ngœi ngalpa ngitha thana

Meriam Mìr
number 1 1-2 2 3
singular ka ---- ma e
non-singular ki mi wa wi, i

Kiwai
number 1 2 3
singular mai rai nowai
dual nimoto rigoto neito
trial nimoibi rigoibi neibi
plural nimo rigo nei

Urradhi
number 1 1-2 2 3
singular ayu(va) ---- antu(va) ulu(va)
dual ampu(la) ali(va) ipu(la) ula(va)
plural (as dual) ana(va) (as dual) (as dual)


Note that most of the Trans Fly languages also have two-gender masculine-feminine systems, though not marked on the pronouns themselves. Wurm 1975:333-334.

However, even though the system has no real surprises for Australian linguistics, it is clear that WCL has innovated in the 1st and 2nd pronouns, which appear to have the following CA origins:

CA *ngali > ngœy [stem: ngœlmu-, ngœimu-] we, not you; and with stem extensions ngalpa you and I/we (old-style singing ngalipa/ngalepa), ngalbai/ngalbe we two (not you), (old style singing ngalebai/ngalibai)


CA *ngana+pulV > ngœba you and I.

The 2nd person dual and plural pronouns are clearly based on forms that literally mean you-two (ngipel) and you-they (ngitha[na]), in the same way as the demonstratives similarly mark the dual and plural (see further below in Nominal Morphology).

The WCL pronouns and their Australian origins:
KLY KulY KY KKY Old KY
(Kowrareg)
Proto-Pama–Nyungan origin
I ngay
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
*ngayi
you and I ngœba ngœba ngœba ngœba ngœba *ngana+pulV
'we two, not you'
we two (not you) ngalbay ngalbai ngalbai/ngalbe ngalbe ngalbe *ngali+[?]
'you and me, you and us'
we (incl. you) ngalpa
stem: ngalpu-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpu-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpu-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpa-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpa-
*ngali+[?]
'you and me, you and us'
we (not you) ngœy
stem: ngœlmu-
ngœi
stem: ngœlmu-
ngœyi
stem: ngœymu-
ngœi
stem: ngœimu-
ngœri
stem: ngœri(mu)-
*ngali
'you and me, you and us'
you (sing.) ni ni ngi ngi ngi *NHina
you two nipel
stem: nipe-
nipel
stem: nipe-
ngipel
stem: ngipe-
ngipel
stem: ngipe-
ngipel
stem: ngipe-
*NHina+pulV
you (pl.) nitha
stem: nithamu-
nitha
stem: nithamu-
ngitha
stem: ngithamu-
ngitha
stem: ngithamu-
ngithana
stem: ngithana(mu)-
*NHina + *DHana 'they'
he nuy
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
*NHu-
she na na na na na *NHan-
they two palay
stem: palamu-
palai
stem: palamu-
pale
stem: palamu-
palai
stem: palamu-,
Boigu pale
stem: palemu-
pale
stem: palamu-
*pula
they thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
*DHana
who nga nga nga nga nga *ngaaNH
what
(no independent nominative-accusative form
mi-,
midhi-
mi- mi- mi- mi- *miNHA 'food' (?)

A 'Pre-Historical' Overview of the Language

An examination of the various sub-systems (vocabulary, syntax, morphology) of WCL gives the following evidence of development:

Papuan (Trans-Fly)
A little basic vocabulary, some abstract vocabulary, some verbs.
Some grammar, such as verb number and different stems for different number forms of some verbs.
Use of state/movement verbs as existential 'to be' verbs.
Two non-personal pronouns
: naag/naga how, namuidh / namuith when (both in KKY, the dialect of the islands off the Papuan coast).

Australian (Paman)
Some basic vocabulary, all personal pronouns (inc. who and what/which), some verbs.
Some grammar, such as nominal and verb morphology (subject, agent, object, genitive,
-l locative, -ka dative in the dialects closer to Australia, perfective, imperfective, -iz(i) perfective intransitive-antipassive. (Note that these typological categories also exist in the Trans-Fly languages; the forms in WCL are clearly Australian).

Austronesian
Some basic vocabulary, terminology dealing with agriculture, canoes, the weather, the sky and the sea, some abstract nouns, some verbs.
Possibly some grammar in the form of function words, such as
waadh (KKY waaza) existential emphasis (i.e. ‘it is true that...’).

The Australian word and structure forms found in WCL give every appearance of being retentions, i.e. inherited, in that the original Australian systems appear to be unchanged at the core level. That is to say, WCL is not a pidgin/creole in origin, but an Australian language which has undergone quite a bit of external lexical and grammatical influence.

Hence, in this respect, WCL appears to be a classic case of shift (Thomason and Kaufmann, 1978:212), whereby speakers of one language, over a long period of time, retained multilingualism while taking over the target language. The Papuan language gave relatively few words to WCL, but changed the originally Australian phonology and syntax profoundly. The contrast of Australian laminal nh/ny and lh/ly and apical n and l has been lost, voicing has become phonemic, and s, z, t, d, o and òò have been introduced. This also seems to have caused reanalysis in the phonology of Australian vocabulary, such that these “foreign” sounds occur in such words.

On the other hand, Austronesian content in WCL (and the neighbouring Papuan languages) appears to be mainly lexicon (including verbs), particularly in the spheres of sea, farming, canoe and sky/weather/astrological terminology, with some possible syntactic words. Hence, this presents a picture (again following Thomason and Kaufmann 1988:212) of a typically extensive borrowing situation with much lexical borrowing and some structural borrowing with a large amount of passive bilingualism with little active bilingualism.

Laade’s picture (1968) of Australian and Papuan settlement in Torres Strait supports the above scenario of Papuan speakers who shifted to an Australian language over a long period of time, and Austronesians who were culturally superstratum, however not in a position to impose their language. He presented folk history evidence that a few Austronesian traders (men) settled at Parema (north-east of Daru) and married local [Proto–Trans Fly speaking] women. To avoid further miscenegenation, they soon moved and settled in Torres Strait, first to the Eastern Islands, then to the Central Islands, then to Moa, Badu and Mabuiag. At Mabuiag, Badu and Moa they found Aboriginal people, killed the men and kept the women (and presumably the children). Some moved on up to Saibai, Doewan and Boeigu to avoid this new miscenegenation, hence the lighter colour of many Saibai, Doewan and Boeigu people. Boeigu folk history collected by Laade also shows direct East Austronesian genetic influence on Boeigu (Laade 1968).

The social milieu that the above discussions presents is that of a few Austronesian men who settled on the outskirts of an East Trans-Fly group, intermarried, and whose children were either bilingual, or speakers of their mothers’ language, with some knowledge of their fathers’ language. The local people did not need to speak the traders’ language, who in turn had to speak the local language. The children in turn would then speak the local language, with varying ability in the fathers’ language, particularly in areas that were culturally important for the fathers.

These people then shifted to Torres Strait - maintaining established ties with Papua - and overlaid an Australian population in such a way that the majority of women spoke an Australian language, with a significant number, mainly men, who spoke a Papuan language with significant Austronesian influence. Over time, the core structure of the mothers’ dominated, with retention of the men’s Papuo-Austronesian content in the appropriate cultural subsystems. In essence this would have been a ‘replay’ of the original settlement by Austronesian traders at Parema, with the women understanding the language of the men, but not really needing to speak it while retaining parts of their language for significant areas. The children then created a new language shift to an Australian language with a Papuan-Austronesian admixture.

Thus, WCL appears to be a mixed language in that a significant part of its lexicon, phonology and grammar is not Australian in origin. The core nominal, pronominal and verb morphology is Australian in both form and grammar - though a certain amount of the grammar is common to Trans-Fly and Paman languages in the first place. Its semantic categories, verb number morphology, and some other morphology are non-Australian in origin. Further, potentially 80% of its word stock is non-Australian. The interplay of the above within the subsystems of WCL lexicon, phonology and grammar points more to mixing through shift and borrowing rather than pidginisation and creolisation.

Outside Influences

The language also has some vocabulary from languages outside the Torres Strait area, from the Indonesian, Malay, Filipino, English and other 'outsiders'. Where loan words from the Western Austronesian (Indonesian, etc.) loans are concerned, it is possible that some such came into the language in pre-European contact days, with the Makassans and similar fishermen/traders who visited northern Australia and Torres Strait.

Examples of probable post-European contact Western Austronesian loan words:
item WCL origin
coconut toddy thúba tuba (Eastern Indonesian or Filipino)
trumps (in cards) záru zaru/jaru (Eastern Indonesian or Filipino)
mate, friend, brother bala
Boigu variants bœra, baya
bela/bala (Eastern Indonesian or Filipino)
bœlasan blachan belacan (Malay)


Some words in the language, assuming that they are Western Austronesian loans, appear to be pre-contact words. This is suggested both by their forms, but also that their use in the language (and in neighbouring languages) seems to indicate this.

Possible pre-European Contact Western Austronesian words in WCL
(Some of these words are ultimately from Arabic, Sanskrit and possibly Portuguese. Ngajedan 1987.)
WCL meaning possible source meaning
aya (KKY)
aye (KLY,KulY,KY)
come! (singular) ayo Malay come!
bayu (KulY,KY)
baaiwa (KLY)
waterspout bayu Malay
(Sanskrit vayu)
wind
ádhi huge, great (also as an honorific)
story (with cultural, religious or similar significance)
'story stone or rock', i.e. a rock or stone that represents or is someone or something with sacred or cultural aignificance, which is explained by the story about the rock or stone
adi Malay
(Sanskrit adhi)
huge, great (also as an honorific)
kœdal(a) crocodile kadal Malay
kadalaq Makasarese
lizard
pawa deed, action, custom paal [pa'al] Malay
(Arabic fa’la
deed, action

In the KKY dialect of WCL, waterspout is markai gùb(a) spirit wind; waterspouts were one of the weapons of the markai who mainly came from the west/north-west i.e. from what is now Indonesia) in the NW monsoon season (when waterspouts are common), and went back to the west/north-west with the SE trades.

The postulation of pawa
as being an early loan from Malay is extremely hypothetical - and suggested not only by the form of the word, but also the loss of the final -l. Two early English loans underwent a similar change, which in essence is a back formation from what in the language appeared to be a plural. Most nouns (a) form the plural with an -l suffix, and (b) in the nominative-accusative singular elide the stem final vowel, as in tukuyapa- same-sex sibling , plural tukuyapal, nominative-accusative tukuyap, and under this model custard-apple became WCL katitap, plural katitapal, and the word mammy-apple (pawpaw/papaya) became WCL mamiyap, plural mamiyapal

Assuming that Malay paal [pa'al] was borrowed as *pawal, with the glottal stop being converted to w, as the language does not have the glottal stop, *pawal would be seen as a plural, with the singular *pawa, under the model (in this case) of kawal
islands, kawa island.

Modern Eastern Austronesian Loans (Polynesian and Melanesian) into the language are mainly of religious or 'academic' use. In general such words are terms for objects that are strictly speaking European goods. One exception is the last in the following table, which has virtually replaced the traditional words imi
spouse’s opposite-sex sibling, opposite sibling’s spouse and ngaubath spouse’s same-sex sibling, same-sex sibling’s spouse. These have also similarly been replaced by the English loan woman in the meaning of sister/daughter-in-law.
WCL meaning origin language meaning in originating language
thúsi book, document, letter, etc. tusi Samoan (same meaning)
laulau table laulau Samoan plaited coconut leaf used as a tray
wakasu anointment oil wakacu Dehu coconut oil
thawiyan
(emotive form thawi)
brother/son-in-law tawean Vanuatu brother-in-law


Other biblical loans are from Greek, Latin and Hebrew:
WCL meaning origin language meaning in originating language
basalaya kingdom basileia Greek id.
aretho holy communion artos Greek id.
Sathana Satan satan Hebrew id.
Sabadh(a) Sunday sabbath Hebrew Saturday (Sabbath)

Dialects

There are four main dialects, two of which are on probably the verge of extinction, one (Kawalgau Ya) through convergence to the neighbouring Kalaw Lagaw Ya. Within the dialects there are two or more subdialects. The average mutual intelligibility rate is around 97%.
  • Northern dialect : Kalau Kawau Ya - Saibai (Saibai Village and Aith, also Bamaga/Seisia on Cape York), Dœwan (Dauan), Bœigu (Boigu)
  • Western dialect : Kalaw Lagaw Ya - Mabuyag (Mabuiag) and Badhu (Badu)
  • Eastern dialect (Central Island dialect) : Kulkalgau Ya - Yama, Waraber, Puruma, Masig and associated islands, now uninhabited, such as Nagi and Gebar
  • Southern Dialect (South-West Islands) : Kawalgau Ya [Kauraraigau Ya] - Muralag, Ngœrupai (alt. Ngurupai, Nœrupai, Nurupai) and the other islands of the Thursday Island group, Mua (alt. Moa), Muri (Mt Adolphus - now uninhabited). Now converging with Kalaw Lagaw Ya (hence the more usual form of the language name is now Kaiwalgau Ya).


The Southern dialect has certain characteristics that link it closely to the northern dialect, and Muralag folk history reflects this, in that the ancestors of the Kowrareg (the Hiamo) originally came from Dharu (Daru, to the north east of Torres Strait) - and who had previously settled on Dharu from Yama in Central Torres Strait [get reference].

Samples of the dialects

When Mum went home, she gave Dad the knife.

Kalau Kawau Ya: Ama na' lagapa uzarima nanga, nadh Babalpa gi [
alt. upi] manu [alt. maninu].

Kalaw Lagaw Ya: Ama na' mudhaka uzarima nanga, nadh Babanika gi [
alt. upi, thurik] manu.

Kulkalgau Ya: Ama na' mudhaka uzarima/uzarimò nanga, nadh Babanika gi [
alt. upi, thurik] manu.

Kawalgau Ya: Ama na' lagapa uzarima nanga, nadha Babanipa gi [
alt. upi, thurik] manul.

(Old Kawalgau Ya (Kowrareg): *Ama na' lagapa[li] uzarrima nanga, nadhu Babanipa[li] gi[ri] [
alt. upi, thurrika] manulai.)

and... simplified ('koinè') Kalaw Lagaw Ya

Ama na' kulai mudh ka uzari, nadh Baban ka gi [
alt. upi, thurik] mani.

Underlying form:

Ama+0 nanga (kul+lai) laga/mudha+pa/ka uzara+i+0+ma nanga,
Mum+NOM REFERENCE (before+LOC) place/shelter+DAT go+ACTIVE+SINGULAR+ACTIVE TODAY PAST REFERENCE

na+dh Baba+ni+pa/ka gi/upi/thurika+0 ma[ni]+0+0+nu.

she+INSTRUMENTAL Dad+LINK+DAT knife+ACC give+ATTAINATIVE+SINGULAR+ATTAINATIVE TODAY PAST

(gi knife, tusk, canine tooth, upi knife made from bamboo, thurik cutting tool)

They cut down a big tree earlier today to make a canoe.

Kalau Kawau Ya: Thana kœi puy pathanu gulpa aymœipa.

Kalaw Lagaw Ya: Thana kœi puuyi pathanu gulka ayimka.

Kulkalgau Ya: Thana kœi puy pathanu[l] gulka aymœika.

Kawalgau Ya: Thana kœi puy pathanu[l] gulpa aymaipa.

(Old Kawalgau Ya (Kowrareg): *Thana kœi puuri pathanulai gulpa[rri] ayimaripa[rri].)

and... simplified ('koinè') Kalaw Lagaw Ya

Thana kulai kœi puy pathai gulka aymaik.

Underlying form:

Thana+0 (kul+lai) kœi puuRi+0 patha+0+0+nu[+lai] gul+ka/pa[ri] ayima+[R]i+ka/pa[ri].
TheyPL+NOM (before+LOC) big tree+ACC chop+ATT+SING+TodayPast[+LOC] canoe+DAT make+VN+Dat

Some Isolect Markers of the 4 dialects of WCL.
Kalau Kawau Ya Kawalgau Ya Kalaw Lagaw Ya Kulkalgau Ya Kauraraigau Ya
(Kowrareg)
you SG ngi ngi ni ni ngi
house laag laag mùùdha mùdh laaga
thunder gigi dhuyum dhuyum dhuyum dhuyum
end, finish muasi-
(B muyasi-)
muasi- minasi- minasi- moasi-
heat kom kœmàn kœmààna kom kœmàna
steam kœman kœmàn kœmààna kœmàn kœmàna
Dative -pa -pa
(-ka)
-ka
(-pa)
-ka
(-pa)
-pa[rri]
(-ka)
Ablative -ngu(z),
-z(i)
-ngu,
-z(i)
-ngu,
-zi
-ngu,
-z(i)
-nguzi,
-zi
Present Perfective
Active Singular
-iz,
-izi(n)
-i,
-izi
-i,
-izi
-i,
-izi
-izi,
-izi(ri)

Dialect Differences

1) Phonology:

Phonological differences between the dialects are amazingly rare - and in general sporadic. The only regular differences are the following:

a) Colloquial final unstressed vowel elision in Kulkalgau Ya and Kawalgau Ya:

maalu sea > maal’

waapi
fish > waap’

thathi
father > thath’

waaru
turtle > waar’

ngadha
appearance, looks > ngadh’

mœràpi
bamboo (à shows the stressed syllable) > mœràp’

bera
rib > ber’

kaba
dance performance > kab’

Such elision is rare or sporadic in the other dialects.

b) Final vowel unstressed vowel devoicing and deletion in Kalaw Lagaw Ya

In Kalaw Lagaw Ya, such final vowels in correct language are devoiced, and deleted in colloquial language, except in a small class of words which include bera
rib, where there is a short vowel in the stem and in which the final vowel is permanently deleted, with compensatory lengthening of the final consonant (thus berr).

Strictly speaking, the process is not final vowel devoicing, but rather stressed vowel lengthening accompanied by final vowel devoicing - except in the case of words such as bera
rib > berr, where the process is final consonant lengthening by the final vowel being 'incorporated' into the consonant. Note that in the following the word-final capital letter represents a devoiced vowel:

maalu
sea > maalU > maal’

waapi
fish > waapI > waap’

thaathi
father > thaathI > thaath’ (Badhu variant thath’)

waaru
turtle > waarU > waar’

ngadha
appearance, looks > ngaadhA > ngaadh’

mœràpi
bamboo (àà shows the stressed syllable) > mœrààpI > mœrààp’

bera
rib > berr

kaba
dance performance > kabb

In declined forms of such words, the long vowel is shortened, and the final vowel voiced, and in words like ber
rib the final vowel often reappears:

maalU
sea + ka dative > maluka

waapI
fish > wapika

thaathI
father > thathika

waarU
turtle > waruka

ngaadhA
appearance, looks > ngadhaka

mœrààpI
bamboo > mœràpika

ber
rib > beraka, berka

kab
dance performance > kabaka, kabka

This vowel shortening in affixed/modified forms exists in all dialects, however the other dialects have retained contrastive length to some extent, whereas Kalaw Lagaw Ya has largely lost it for ‘morphophonological’ length, where the stressed vowel in non-emotive words (see below) of one or two syllables is automatically lengthened in the nominative-accusative; this also applies to words of three syllables with second syllable stress (as in mœrààpI bamboo).

One of the very few length contrasts in the Kalaw Lagaw Ya dialect is kaabA
knot in bamboo etc. vs kab dance performance (kab in Old Kawalgaw Ya was kœRaba, and œRa has regularly given short a in Kalaw Lagaw Ya); such length contrasts are more widespread in the other dialects.

The exceptions are (1) the small class or words that include ber
rib and kab dance performance, and (2) emotive words. Emotive words are those that equate to a certain extent to diminutives in languages such as Irish, Dutch and German, where specific suffixes are added to show 'diminutive' status (-ín, -Cje and -chen respectively). Emotive words in the Kalaw Lagaw Ya dialect include familiar kinship terms [the equivalent of English Mum, Dad and the like] and words used in emotive contexts such as singing/poetry.
Word Non-Emotive Emotive
Mum (apùùwA, apùù, àpu - mother) Ama
Dad (thaathI, thaath - father) Baba
Child kaazI, kaaz kazi
Wife iipI, iip ipi
Home (Island) laagA, laag laga
Dust, Spray pœœyA, pœœy pœya (also paya)
Bamboo mœrààpI, mœrààp mœràpi (also marapi)
Head kuwììkU, kuwììk kuwìku, kuiku


c) Kalau Kawau Ya final i-glide deletion

A small class of words in KKY lose the final i-glide found in the other dialects, including the following:

banana plant : KLY/KulY/KY dawai, KKY dawa

spot, stain : KLY/KulY/KY burkui (bœrkui), KKY bœrku (burku)

blank skink : KLY/KulY/KY mogai, KKY Saibai/Dœwan mogo, Bœigu moga

old : KLY/KulY/KY kulbai, KKY kulba

a short while, first before doing something else : KLY/KulY/KY mamui, KKY mamu

birth cord : KLY/KulY/KY kùpai, KKY kùpa

Word forms in neighbouring languages as well in the Kauraraigau Ya (Kowrareg) of the mid-to-late 19th century, such as the Meriam Mìr kopor and Kauraraigau Ya kupar/kopar
birth cord show that in such words the final -i/Ø are the modern forms of older *ɾ.

2) Syntax

The main syntactic differences are:

a) Verb negative construction:

In all dialects except Kalau Kawau Ya, the verb negative is the nominalised privative form of the verbal noun. As this form in itself a noun, its subject and direct object are cast in the genitive:

Ngath waapi purthanu
I ate a fish

Ngai stuwaka uzarima
I went to the store

Ngau wapiu purthaiginga
I didn’t eat a fish

Ngau stuwaka uzaraiginga
I didn’t go to the store

The Kalau Kawau Ya dialect uses the verbal noun privative form as an invariable verb negative:

Ngath waapi purthanu
I ate a fish

Ngai stuwapa uzarima
I went to the store

Ngath waapi purthaiginga
I didn’t eat a fish

Ngai stuwapa uzaraiginga
I didn’t go to the store

b) Verb Tenses/Aspects

The Kalau Kawau Ya dialect has the tenses and aspects listed in the section on verb morphology. The other dialects have largely lost the remote future tense, using the habitual instead; the remote future in the other dialetcs is retained most commonly as a 'future imperative', where the imperative refers to a vague period in the future. The Kalaw lagaw Ya dialect also has a 'last night' tense, where the adverb bungil last night has become grammaticalised as a verb ending, following the example of the adverb ngùl yesterday, which had previously become grammaticalised as a 'recent past' tense marker in all dialects, with reduction to -ngu in the Kalau kawau Ya dialect. In the other dialects bongel last night is a fully functioning temporal adverb used in conjunction with either the today past or the recent past.

In the case of the shape of affixes, dialects differ in the following:

1) present imperfective/near future perfective/verbal noun dative:

KKY/KY -pa, KLY/KulY -ka

2) Recent past

KKY -ngu, KLY/KY/KulY -ngul

3) Today past

KKY/KLY/KulY -nu, KY -nul (older -nulai)

4) Habitual

KKY -paruig/paruidh/-parui/-paru/-pu (-pu most commonly on stems of two or more syllables, and the bi-syllabic forms on stems of one syllable [the consonant final forms are emphatic forms])

KLY/KulY -kuruig

KY -kurui

c) Nominal Affixes

The main nominal affix difference is the dative ending, which has the following forms in the various dialects:

KLY/KulY -ka; -pa with kipa to here and sipa to there; -pa sometimes in poetry/singing

KY -pa; -ka in ngaikika
to/for/towards me; -ka often in poetry/singing.

KKY -pa in all cases; -ka often in poetry/singing.

The plural/HAVE suffix -LAI also shows a small amount of dialect variation with stems of two syllables, where Kulkalgau Ya differs from the other dialects in retaining the full form form of the suffix -LAI, reduced to -L in the other dialects. In stems of three or more syllables, the suffix is reduced to -L in all dialects, while retained as -LAI (variants according to noun sub-class -THAI, -AI, -DAI) with stems of one syllable.

Three+ syllable stem:
burum pig, stem: buruma-, plural burumal

Bisyllabic stem:
lag, KLY laagA
place, home, home island, stem: laga-, plural lagal, KulY lagalai

monosyllabic stems: all dialects identical

(1) Regular vowel final:
ma
spider, stem: ma-, plural malai

(2) Regular -i glide final:
mui
fire, stem: mui-, plural muithai

(3) Regular -l final:
gul
sailing canoe, stem: gul-, plural gulai

(4) Regular -r final:
wœr/wur/uur
water, stem: wœr-/wur-/ur-, plural wœlai/wulai/ulai, KKY wœrai

(5) Irregular vowel final stem:
ya
speech, word(s), message, language, etc., stem: ya-, plural yadai

d) Vocabulary

The main differences between the dialects are to do with vocabulary, as can be seen in the following examples:

house/building : KLY mùùdha (laaga), KulY mùdh (laag), KY laag (mùdh), KKY laag

mud : KLY/KulY/KY berdhar (sœœya sandy mud/silt), KKY sœœi (berdhar softness of food, mud, etc.)

grandad : KLY/KulY/KY athe, KKY pòpu

frog : KLY/KulY kœtaaka, kœtak, KY kat, KLY (Saibai-Dœwan) kat, (Bœigu) kœteku

axe : KLY/KulY/KY aga, KKY agathurik (thurik cutting tool)

namesake : KLY/KulY natham, KKY/KY nasem

small, little : KLY/KulY/KY mœgi, Saibai/Dœwan mœgina, Bœigu mœgina, kœthuka

woman, female : KKY yipkaz/yœpkaz [stem yipkazi-/yœpkazi-], KLY/KulY ipikaz (KLY variant iipka) [stem ipkazi-], KY ipkai [stem ipkazi-]

man, male : KKY garkaz [stem garkazi-], KLY/KulY garka [stem garkazi-], KY garkai [stem garkazi-]

unmarried young/teenage woman : KKY ngawakaz [stem ngawakazi-], KLY/KulY ngawka/ngoka [stem ngawkazi-/ngokazi-], KY ngawakai [stem ngawakazi-]

song : KLY naawu (plural nawul], KulY nawu (plural nawulai), KY nawu (plural nawul), KKY na (plural nathai)

moon, month : KLY kisaayi, poetry mœlpal, KulY/KY kiisay, poetry mœlpal, KKY mœlpal, poetry kiisay

Phonology

Kala Lagaw Ya is the only Australian language to have the alveolar
Alveolar consonant
Alveolar consonants are articulated with the tongue against or close to the superior alveolar ridge, which is called that because it contains the alveoli of the superior teeth...

 fricatives
Fricative consonant
Fricatives are consonants produced by forcing air through a narrow channel made by placing two articulators close together. These may be the lower lip against the upper teeth, in the case of ; the back of the tongue against the soft palate, in the case of German , the final consonant of Bach; or...

 /s/ and /z/. However, these have allophonic variants tʃ and dʒ, which are the norm in Australia languages (but non-contrasting). These latter two are allophones in that in all environments /s/ and /z/ can appear, while /tʃ/ and /dʒ/ can not appear at the end of a word. All the stops, except for the alveolars t and d, have fricative allophones, thus p can be [p] or [ɸ], k can be [k] or [x], b [b] or [β], and so on. Furthermore, it is one of the few Australian languages with fully functioning voiced-voiceless distinctions (p/b, t/d, s/z, k/g, th/dh) - and one of the few without retroflex
Retroflex consonant
A retroflex consonant is a coronal consonant where the tongue has a flat, concave, or even curled shape, and is articulated between the alveolar ridge and the hard palate. They are sometimes referred to as cerebral consonants, especially in Indology...

 stops.

The language is also one of the few Australian languages with only one rhotic, one l and one n. The earliest recorded dialect, Kawalgau Ya (Kauraraigau Ya / Kowrareg), however, did have two rhotics, the tap and the glide; the rhotic glide has in general become /j/, /w/ or zero in the other dialects (and Modern Kawalgau Ya), rarely /r/. Neighbouring languages retain an /r/ in related words, such as;

sayima outrigger :
Old Kawalgaw Ya sarima, Kiwai (Papua) harima, Gudang (Australia) charima

babath
opposite-sex sibling :
Old Kawalgau Ya bœrabatha
opposite-sex sibling, Meriam Mìr berbet sibling.

It is interesting to note, however, that in singing, s, z and r are pronounced [s], [z], and [ɽ] (the glide), and virtually never as /tʃ/, /dʒ/ and /r/.
Consonants voiceless
stop/
(af)fricative
voiced
stop/
(af)fricative
nasal approximants
lateral
rhotic
labial
Labial
Labial may refer to:*the lips*the labia *In linguistics, a labial consonant*In zoology, the labial scales...

p b m w
dental th dh n l
alveolar
Alveolar consonant
Alveolar consonants are articulated with the tongue against or close to the superior alveolar ridge, which is called that because it contains the alveoli of the superior teeth...

t d - r
alveo-palatal s z - y
velar
Velar consonant
Velars are consonants articulated with the back part of the tongue against the soft palate, the back part of the roof of the mouth, known also as the velum)....

k g ng -

Vowels -round +round
+high,-mid i,ii u,uu
+high,+mid e,ee ù,ùù
-high,+mid œ,œœ o,oo
-high,-mid a,aa ò,òò


Notes:

a) The consonant d varies to some extent with r, particularly in KKY/KY kadai-/karai-, KLY/KulY kad[a] upwards.

b) The typewritten forms of œ and œœ are oe and ooe.

c) The long vowel ùù is only found in Kala Lagaw Ya.

d) Length is to a certain extent contrastive, and partly allophonic.

e) The +/-round contrast is reminiscent of Papuan phonology.

The mid long vowels are allophonic variants of the mid short vowels that are in the process of developing phonemic status, while the short vowel ò is similarly in origin an allophone of òò.

Internal reconstruction and comparison with neighbouring languages suggests an underlying four vowel structure with contrasting vowel length, where underlying *i typically gives surface i and e, underlying *a typically gives surface a and œ, underlying *ò typically gives surface o and ù, and underlying *u typically gives surface ù and u (there are other realisations as well, depending on rules of assimilation etc.):
Underlying Vowels -round +round
+high *i,*ii *u,*uu
-high *a,*aa *ò,*òò


The language undergoes low-level vowel shifts, caused by stress domination within clauses. Long vowels are shortened, and short vowels raise when the word is preceded by morphemes such as adjectives, demonstrative articles, prefixes and the like; the changes also change within words when these are suffixed:

laag place - senabi lag that place

lagal
places - sethabi lœgal those places (also sethabi lagal)

mœrap
bamboo - mœrœpil bamboo plants/poles/sticks (also mœrapil)

guul
sailing canoe - senaubi gul that canoe

thonaral
times - sethabi thunaral those times (also sethabi thonaral)

zageth
work - zagithapa to/for work [dative] (also zagethapa) (zageth is a compound of za thing + geth hand)

The processes are low-level in that they are not 'automatic' - the changes do not have to occur and can be consciously 'blocked'. In normal speech, vowel shortening and the change of a to œ are the norm, which the changes of e to i and o to u are sporadic, and most common in unstressed syllables.

Assimilation of vowels to other vowels in the vicinity and consonants is also widespread, particularly of the vowel œ:

wœrab coconut - wurab - urab

yœlpai
lead [verbal noun] - yilpai - ilpai

ngœnu
whose - ngunu

kœu
belonging to here - kou

ngœba
you and I - ngaba

Orthography

There is no strict standard spelling, and three slightly different orthographies (and often mixes of them) are in use.

The three spelling systems used for the language:

The Mission Spelling (established at first by Loyalty Islands
Loyalty Islands
The Loyalty Islands are an archipelago in the Pacific. They are part of the French territory of New Caledonia, whose mainland is away. They form the Loyalty Islands Province , one of the three provinces of New Caledonia...

 missionaries in 1870s, then modified by Polynesian missionaries in the 1880s) :
a, b, d, e, g, i, j, k, l, m, n, ng, o, ö, p, r, s, t, u, z, sometimes also th, dh, dth, tr, dr, oe, ë, w, y, j, and sometimes double vowels to show length. This spelling system was strongly inspired by the one used for the drehu (Lifu) language in the very early period, though later with the change of non-European Mission personnel from Lifu to Polynesian, as well as the growing number of indigenous Torres Strait missionaries, the spelling system lost the overtly Drehu forms tr, dr and ë, which had no phonological basis in Kala Lagaw Ya. The mission system is the orthography used in the Reports of the Cambridge Expedition to the Torres Strait (Haddon et al., 1898 and on, University of Cambridge) and in "Myths and Legends of Torres Strait" (Lawrie, University of Queensland, 1971). Ray, the linguist of the Cambridge Expedition, also used various diacrtitics to represent vowel length and quality.

Klokheid and Bani (established 1970s) : a, aa, b, d (alveolar), dh (dental), e, ee, g, i, ii, k, l, m, n, ng, o, oo,oe (/ə/), ooe (/əː/), p, r, s, t (alveolar), th (dental), u, uu, w, y, z

Saibai, Boigu, Dauan students (established late 1970s) :
a, b, d (alveolar), dh (dental), e, g, i, k, l, m, n, ng, o, oe (/ə/), p, r, s, t (alveolar), th (dental), u, w, y, z (vowel length is not represented).

People not only use these three slightly differing spelling systems, but they also write words more or less as they pronounce them. Because of this words are often spelt in various ways, for example sena/sina
that, there, kothai/kothay/kothei/kothey/kothe back of head, occiput. Variation like this depends on age, family, island, and other factors such as poetic speech. It can be difficult at times to decide which is most correct - different people have different opinions (and sometimes have very strong opinions).

Though in general the pronunciation of older people has priority, some people can actually get quite offended if they think the language is written the ‘wrong’ way. Some insist that the mission spelling should be used, others the Bani spelling, and still others the KKY (Saibai etc.) spelling, and still again others use mixes of two or three, or adaptations thereof. Some writers of the Mabuiag-Badhu dialect (Kala Lagaw Ya), for example, write mainly in the Mission system, sometimes use the diagraphs oe, th, dh (variant dth) and sometimes use capital letters at the ends of words to show devoiced vowels, such as ngukI fresh water/drinking water, fruit juice /ŋʊːki̥/. In the Bani/Klokheid orthograophy nguki is written nguuki, and in the other dialects the final vowel is either fully voiced, nguki /ŋʊki/), or elided, nguk /ŋʊk/).

The biggest bone of contention between the advocates of the 'modern' orthographies and the 'traditionalist' orthographies is the use of w and y to show the semi-vowels (or semi-consonants if you prefer). In general native speakers in literacy classes seem to find y and w very difficult to learn, and that u and i are the 'logical' letters to use. Getting untrained speakers to break up words by sound or syllable suggests that u and i are really the underlying sounds. Thus, a word like dhaudhai mainland, continent syllabifies as dha-u-dha-i, not dhau-dhai. In songs, the glide-u/i can also be given full syllable status. 'Historical' considerations also point to the semi-vowels often being vocalic rather than consonantal. Thus, lagau, the genitive of laag[a] place is in underlying form laaga+ngu; the full form of the genitive ending -ngu is only retained where the nominal has a monosyllabic stem (see the section on Nominal Morphology). Similarly, verbal nouns end in -i, e.g. lumai, stem luuma- search, look for, seek, hunt. The earliest records of the language of the mid-19th century show that the verbal noun ending was previously -ri (thus lumari), where the -r- was presumably the rhotic glide rather than the rhotic tap/trill.

A dictionary now in preparation (Mitchell/Ober) uses a orthography based on detailed study of the surface and underlying phonology of the language, as well as on observation of how people write in real life situations. It is a mix of the Mission and Kalau Kawau Ya orthographies with the addition of diacritics (the letters in brackets) to aid correct pronunciation, since many of the people who will use this dictionary will not be speakers of the language:

a (á), b, d, dh, e (é), g, i (í), k, l, m, n, ng, o (ó, ò, ò'), œ (œ'), r, s, t, th, u (ú, ù), w, y, z

Within this orthography, w and y are treated as consonants - this is their phonogical status in the language - while u and i are used as the glides where phonological considerations show that the 'diphthong' combination has vocalic status.

Pronunciation of the letters

The English pronunciations given in the list below are those of Australian English, and are only meant as a guide. The letters in square brackets ([]) are the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA).

a (short) [a] : ‘u’ as in hut - gath shallow, shallows, mathaman hit, kill

a, á (long) (aa in the Bani orth.) [aː] ‘a’ as in
father - áth bottom turtle shell (plastron), ma spider, lág place

b [b] as in English - Báb
Dad, bœbu current, stream, bibir power, authority

d [d] as in English - da
chest, idi grease, fat, dead-calm sea

dh [d̪] similar to d, but with the tip of the tongue put against the top teeth- dha
ladder, stairs, adhal outside, Bádhu Badu

e (short) [e] ‘e’ as in
bed - bero rib, nge then, tete animal/bird leg

e, é (long) (ee in the Bani orth.) [eː] ‘are’ as in
bared - gér sea snake, dhe slime, sei there

g [ɡ] as in English
get, never as in general - gigi thunder, gág mangrove swamp

i (short) [i] short ‘ee’ as in
feet - midh how, sisi gecko, ipi wife

i, í (long) (ii in the Bani orth.) [iː] ‘ee’ as in
feed - síb liver, gi knife, ígil life

k [k] as in English - kikiman
hurry up, kakayam bird-of-paradise

l [l] similar to English ‘l’ in
lean, but with the tip of the tongue against the top teeth; never as in English kneel - lág place, home, li basket, gúl double-outrigger sailing canoe

m [m] as in English - mám
love, affection, Ama Mum, Aunty, ma spider

n [n] similar to English ‘n’ in
nun, but with the tip of the tongue against the top teeth - na song, nan her, nanu her(s)

ng [ŋ] as in English
sing; never as in English finger - ngai I, me, ngœrang armpit

o (short) [o] more or less ‘o’ as is in
got, though more rounded - sob slowness, mogo blank skink

o (long) (oo in the Bani orth.) [oː] more or less ‘o’ as in
god, though more rounded - gor tie-hole, so show

ò (short) [ɔ] short version of ‘oa’ in
broad - mòdhabil costs, prices, gòyal bald

ò (long) (oo in the Bani orth.) [ɔː] ‘oa’ in
broad - mòs lung, spittle, gòy baldness

œ (short) [ə] ‘a’ as in
about - bœtœm lean (animals), bœga mallard

œ (long) (ooe in the Bani orth.) [əː] more or less like ‘er’ in
herd - wœr water, Wœy Venus, bœi coming

p [p] as in English - papi
noose, trap, áp garden, Pòpu Grandad

r [r] similar to ‘tt’ in
better when said fast (that is to say, when better is pronounced ‘bedder’). Before another consonant and at the end of a word, it is often trilled (like in ‘stage’ Scottish English or 'rr' in Spanish). In singing, however, it is normally pronounced much like the American English ‘r’ - ári rain, louse, rùg rag, piece of cloth, ár dawn

s [s] most commonly like English ‘s’ in
sister; sometimes like English ‘ch’ in chew when at the beginning of a word or in the middle of a word; never like ‘s’ in ‘as’ (which is a ‘z’ sound) - sas style, showing off, sisi gecko, sagul game, fun, dance

t [t] as in English - tádu
sand-crab, tídan return, understand, ít rock oyster

th [t̪] similar to t, but with the tip of the tongue put against the top teeth - tha
crocodile tail, thathi father, geth hand

u (short) [u] short ‘u’ as in
lute - buthu sand, gulai sailing canoes

u, ú (long) (uu in the Bani orth.) [uː] ‘oo’ in
woo - búzar fat, blubber, thu smoke

ù [ʊ] ‘u’ as in
put - mùdh shelter, kùt late afternoon, early evening, kùlai first, before

w [w] Not as strong as English ‘w’ in
we ; for most speakers of the language the only difference between w and short u is that w is shorter - wa yes, kawa island, báw wave

y [j] Not as strong as English ‘y’ in
yes; for most speakers of the language the only difference between y and short i is that y is much shorter - ya speech, talk, language, aya come!, máy well, spring; tears; pearl-shell, nacre

z [z] most commonly like English ‘z’ in
zoo, or English ‘s’ in has; sometimes like English ‘j’ in jump, or ‘dg’ in budge when at the beginning or in the middle of a word - zázi grass skirt, za thing, object, zizi crackle, crack, rustling noise
Combinations of vowels (‘diphthongs’, such as ai, au, œi, eu etc.) are pronounced as written. Thus, for example, ai is a-i (basically very similar to ‘i’ in ‘mine’ with a posh accent). In singing and sometimes in slow speech, such vowel combinations can be said separately. The diphthongs are:

ei - sei
there

iu - biuni
kookoobuura, kingfisher

œi - bœi
coconut frond

eu - seu
belonging to there

ai - Saibai
Saibai

œu - kœubu
battle, war

òi - òi
hoy!, hey! (reply to a call, vocative particle)

au - kaub
tiredness

ui - mui
fire

ou - berou
of a/the rib

In the Bani and Saibai (etc.) orthographies, these are written as follows:

ey - sey

iw - biwni

œy - bœy

ew - sew

ay - Saybay

œw - kœwbu

oy - oy

aw - kawb

uy - muy

ow - berow

Nominal Morphology

Where the morphology is concerned, the language is somewhere along the continuum between agglutinative and declining. Nominals have the following cases: nominative, accusative, instrumental (subsumes ergative), dative (subsumes allative), ablative (subsumes elative), specific locative, nonspecific locative (subsumes perlative and comitative), and global locative. Nominals also have the following derived forms: negative, similative, proprietive (which also forms the noun nominative-accusative plural) and resultative. All stems end in a vowel or a semi-vowel, except for a few monosyllables ending in -r and -l (which includes a very few reduplicated words, like tharthar boiling, seething, as well as ngipel you two [a compound of ngi you [singular] and -pal two]). For many nouns the surface nominative(-accusative) undergoes a final stem-vowel deletion rule; in the Kalaw Lagaw Ya dialect the rule results in final devoiced vowels accompanied by main vowel lengthening. There are three numbers, singular, dual and plural. Singular and dual are the same form in all nominals except the personal pronouns. Furthermore, the plural is only distinguished in the nominative-accusative - except for the personal pronouns, where the difference in number is shown by the stem.

There are two nominal classes, Common Nominals (common nouns, demonstratives, locative/temporal/etc. adverbs) and Proper Nominals (Proper names [personal names, boat names, emotive kinship terms], pronouns). The major difference between the two classes are 1) semantic - Proper Nominals have pronominal characteristics, and, 2) declensional, for example Proper Nominals have one locative case rather than the three of Common Nominals (see further the declensions below).

Note that the following are in the Kalau Kawau Ya dialect.

Common nominal declensions
Case/Suffix Hoe/Adze Place/Home Knife Water Mud Middle looking giving, getting, being, moving, doing, etc.
stem type multisyllabic
-u final
multisyllabic monosyllabic
vowel final
monosyllabic
-r/-l final
monosyllabic
-i glide final
locative nominal
('adverb')
multisyllabic
verbal noun
monosyllabic
verbal noun
stem pábu- lága- gi- wœr- sái- dhadha- naga+i- má+i-
nom-acc. sing.-dual pábu lág gi wœr sái dhadh
(compounds only)
nœgai mái
nom-acc. pl. pabul lagal gilai wœrai saithai dhadhal
(compounds only)
--- ---
inst. pabun lagan ginu/gín wœrnu/wœran saithu dhadhan nœgain main
gen. pabu lagau gingu wœrngu saingu dhadhau nœgai mai
(maingu)
dat. pabupa lagapa gipa wœrpa saipa dhadhapa nœgaipa maipa
abl. pabungu lagangu gingu wœrngu saingu dhadhaz nœgaile maithaile
sp.loc. pabunu,
pabu'
laganu,
laga'
gilai,
ginu
wœrai,
wœrnu
saithai,
saithe
dhadhal,
dhadha'
nœgainu mainu
n-sp-loc pabuya lagaya giya wœriya saiya dhadhaya nœgaiya maiya
gl-loc. pabuyab lagayab gipu wœrab, wœrpu saiyab, saipu dhadhayab nœgaiya maiya
prop. pabul(ai) lagal(ai) gilai wœrai saithai,
saithe
dhadhal(ai)
(compounds only)
nœgail(ai) maithai
priv. pabugi lagagi gigi wœrgi saigi dhadhagi
(compounds only)
nœgaigi maigi
sim. pabudh(a) lagadh(a) gidha wœrdha/wœradh saidh(a) dhadhadh(a)
(compounds only)
nœgaidh(a) maidh(a)
res. pabuzi lagazi gizi wœrzi saizi dhadhazi
(compounds only)
nœgaizi maizi


There are few irregular nouns, the most common being

(1) ái food, yá speech, language, message, etc., li basket, lú mound, bump, hump

instrumental aidu, yadu, lidu, ludu
specific locative/proprietive-plural aidai/aide, yadai, lidai, ludai

(2) KKY ná, KLY naawu, KulY/KY nawu song

instrumental KKY nathu, KLY/KulY/KY nathun, nawun

specific locative/proprietive-plural KKY nathai, KLY/KY nawul, KulY nawlai

(3) zá thing, object, matter, etc.

This word has a fuller stem form, zapu-, which appears in certain forms:

instrumental zapun
genitive zapu
proprietive-plural zapul

In the locative forms both stems (za- and zapu-) appear:

specific locative zanu, zapunu, etc.

(4) gœiga sun, day; bireg/bereg shelf

The stems of these words have different forms to the nominative-accusative-

gœiga - stem:gœigœyi-, gœigi-; bireg/bereg - stem:bœreigi-

Demonstratives

The language has a closed class of demonstrative morphemes with special morphological characteristics:

Prefixes:

pi there in the distance in a specific position

kai there in the distance in a non-specific position

Stems:

ka, í here, this

se/si there, that (not too far away)

gui, mulu/ngùl down there

ka, karai/kadai up there (variant forms of the one underlying stem)

ngapa there beyond

pai, pa, paipa ahead there, up close there (variant forms of the one underlying stem *pai)

pun[i], pawa off from there, back from there, back over there, back there (possibly variant forms of the one underlying stem)

These demonstratives (as stem forms) can have masculine, feminine and non-singular forms (and as such are pronominal) as well as case forms. Í here, this and se/si there, that (not too far away) take the gender/number morphemes as suffixes, and the other demonstratives take them as prefixes. Note that ka non-specifically here and kai there in the distance in a non-specific position cannot appear with the gender/number morphemes, as these latter are specific by their nature. Í and se/si also take an article forming affix -bi to become demonstrative articles (e.g. senaubi kaz that boy, senabi kaz that girl, sepalbi kaz those two children, sethabi kœzil those children.)

Ka, í and se/si:
Case/Suffix here
non specific
here
specific
there
non-specific
there
specific
nom-acc kai in mas.,
ina fem.,
ipal dual,
itha pl.
sei,
senau mas.,
sena/sina fem.,
sepal/sipal dual,
setha/sitha pl
inst. kedha --- kedha ---
gen. kœu --- seu ---
dat. kœpa --- sepa/sipa ---
abl. kœzi --- seizi/sizi ---
sp.loc. kai in mas.,
ina fem.,
ipal dual,
itha pl.
sei/si senau mas.,
sena/sina fem.,
sepal/sipal dual,
setha/sitha pl.
n-sp-loc/gl.loc kaiki inuki mas.,
inaki fem.,
ipalki dual,
ithaki pl.
seiki/siki senauki mas.,
senaki/sinaki fem.,
sepalki/sipalki dual,
sethaki/sithaki pl.
sim. kedha kedha kedha kedha
article kedhabi inubi mas.,
inabi fem.,
ipalbi dual,
ithabi pl.
kedhabi senaubi mas.,
senabi/sinabi fem.,
sepalbi/sipalbi dual,
sethabi/sithabi pl.


Other demonstratives:
Case/Suffix gui ka(rai) ngapa pai/pa pun/pawa
nom-acc-inst-sp.loc
specific
(forms without the pi prefix are more pronominal in function)
(pi)nugui mas.,
(pi)nagui fem.,
(pi)palgui dual,
(pi)thagui pl.
(pi)nuka mas.,
(pi)naka fem.,
(pi)palka dual,
(pi)thaka pl.
(pi)nungap mas.,
(pi)nangap fem.,
(pi)palngap dual,
(pi)thangap pl.
(pi)nupai mas.,
(pi)napai fem.,
(pi)palpai dual,
(pi)thapai pl.
(pi)nupun mas.,
(pi)napun fem.,
(pi)palpun dual,
(pi)thapun pl.
nom-acc-inst-loc
non-specific
kaigui kaika kaingap kaipai/kaipaipa kaipun, kaipawapa
specific dat.
(forms without the pi prefix are more pronominal in function)
(pi)numulupa mas.,
(pi)namulupa fem.,
(pi)palmulupa dual,
(pi)thamulupa pl.
(pi)nukaripa mas.,
(pi)nakaripa fem.,
(pi)palkaripa dual,
(pi)thakaripa pl.
(pi)nungapapa mas.,
(pi)nangapapa fem.,
(pi)palngapapa dual,
(pi)thangapapa pl.
(pi)nupaipa mas.,
(pi)napaipa fem.,
(pi)palpaipa dual,
(pi)thapaipa pl.
(pi)nupawapa mas.,
(pi)napawapa fem.,
(pi)palpawapa dual,
(pi)thapawapa pl.
non-specific dat. mulupa karaipa/kadaipa kaingapapa (kai)paipa (kai)pawapa
abl. kizigui kizika kizingap kizipai kizipun
n-sp-loc/gl.loc neutral
(forms without the pi prefix are more pronominal in function)
(pi)nuguiki mas., (
pi)naguiki fem.,
(pi)palguiki dual,
(pi)thaguiki pl.
(pi)nukaki mas.,
(pi)nakaki fem.,
(pi)palkaki dual,
(pi)thakaki pl.
(pi)nungapaki mas.,
(pi)nangapaki fem.,
(pi)palngapaki dual,
(pi)thangapaki pl.
(pi)nupaiki/(pi)nupaipa mas.,
(pi)napaiki/(pi)napaipa fem.,
(pi)palpaiki/(pi)palpaipa dual,
(pi)thapaiki/(pi)thapaipa pl.
(pi)nupuniki/(pi)nupawapa mas.,
(pi)napuniki/(pi)napawapa fem.,
(pi)palpuniki/(pi)palawapa dual,
(pi)thapuniki/(pi)thapawapa pl.
n-sp/gl-loc kaiguiki kaikaki kaingapaki kaipaiki/kaipaipa kaipunki, kaipawapa


Pronouns : singular

Note that the third person pronouns are also used as definite articles, e.g. Nuidh garkœzin nan yipkaz imadhin The man saw the woman.
Case/Suffix I/me you he/it (the) she/it (the) who what
nom. ngai ngi nui na nga mi- (miai, miza)
acc. ngœna ngin nuin nan ngan mi-
(miai, miza);
min
inst. ngath ngidh nuidh nadh ngadh midh
(miaidu/miden/midu/midun, mizœpun)
gen. ngau (mas.), ngœzu (fem.) nginu nungu nanu ngœnu mingu
(miaingu, mizœngu)
dat. ngayapa ngibepa nubepa nabepa ngabepa mipa
(miaipa, mizœpa)
abl. ngaungu(z) (mas.), ngœzungu(z) (fem.) nginungu(z) nungungu(z) nanungu(z) ngœnungu(z) mingu(zi)
(miaingu, mizœngu)
sp.loc. ngaibiya ngibiya nubiya nabiya ngabiya miaide/miainu,
mizœpunu
n-sp-loc ngaibiya ngibiya nubiya nabiya ngabiya miaiya,
mizœpuya
gl-loc. ngaibiya ngibiya nubiya nabiya ngabiya miaiyab,
mizœpuyab
proprietive/plural ---------- ---------- ---------- ---------- ---------- midel, mizœpul
priv. ngaugi (mas.), ngœzugi (fem.) nginugi nungugi nanugi ngœnugi miaigi,
mizœgi
sim. ngaudh (mas.), ngœzudh (fem.) nginudh nungudh nanudh ngœnudh midh
(miaidh, mizœpudh)
res. ---------- ---------- ---------- ---------- ---------- miaizi, mizœzi


Personal pronouns : dual
Case/Suffix we two you and I you two them two
(the two)
who-two
nom-acc-inst ngalbe ngœba ngipel palai
(Boigu pale)
ngawal
gen. ngalben ngœban ngipen palamun
(Boigu palemun)
(as for singular)
dat. ngalbelpa ngœbalpa ngipelpa palamulpa
(Boigu palemulpa)
(as for singular)
abl. ngalbelngu ngœbalngu ngipelngu palamulngu
(Boigu palemulngu)
(as for singular)
loc. ngalbeniya ngœbaniya ngipeniya palamuniya
(Boigu palemuniya)
(as for singular)
sim. ngalbedh ngœbadh ngipedh palamudh
(Boigu palemudh)
(as for singular)


Ngawal who two is constructed from nga who plus the clitic -wal both, and two (dual conjunction).

Personal pronouns : plural
Case/Suffix we (not you) we (inc. you) you they (the) who
nom-acc-inst ngœi ngalpa ngitha thana ngaya
gen. ngœimun ngalpan ngithamun thanamun (as for singular)
dat. ngœimulpa ngalpalpa ngithamulpa thanamulpa (as for singular)
abl. ngœimulngu ngalpalngu ngithamulngu thanamulngu (as for singular)
loc. ngœimuniya ngalpaniya ngithamuniya thanamuniya (as for singular)
sim. ngœimudh ngalpadh ngithamudh thanamudh (as for singular)


Ngaya who many is constructed from nga who plus the clitic -ya and others (plural conjunction).

Personal names and familiar kinship terms
(Familiar kinship terms are the equivalent of English kin terms such as Dad and Mum, while non-familiar terms are the equivalent of Father and Mother; these latter are treated as common nouns in the language)
Case/Suffix Tom (mas.) Anai (fem.) Dad/Uncle
(cf. father/uncle)
Mum/Aunty
(cf. mother/aunt
nom-inst Tom Anai Báb
(thathi)
Ama
(ápu)
acc-gen. Toman Anaina Baban
(thathiu)
Amana
(apuwau)
dat. Tomalpa Anailpa Babalpa
(thathipa)
Amalpa
(apuwapa)
abl. Tomalngu Anailngu Babalngu
(thathingu)
Amalngu
(apuwangu)
loc. Tomaniya Anainiya Babaniya
(thathiya)
Amaniya
(apuwaya)
proprietive/plural ----------- ----------- babal
(thathil)
amal
(apuwal)
priv. ----------- ----------- babagi
(thathigi)
amagi
(apuwagi)
sim. Tomadh Anaidh babadh
(thathidh)
amadh
(apuwadh)
res. ----------- ----------- babazi
(thathizi)
amazi
(apuwazi)

Verb Morphology

Verbs can have over 100 different aspect, tense, voice, mood and number forms. Verb agreement is with the object (i.e. 'ergative') in transitive clauses, and with the subject in intransitive clauses. Imperatives, on the other hand, agree with both subject and object in transitive clauses.

There are three aspects (perfective, imperfective, habitual), two voices (active [which focuses on the verb activity and subsumes many intransitives, many antipassives and some transitives] and attainative [which subsumes many transitives, some antipassives and some intransitives]), two moods (non-imperative and imperative [which resembles a subjunctive in some uses], 6 tenses (remote future, today future, present, today past, recent past, remote past) and four numbers (singular, dual, specific plural, animate active plural - in form the animate active plural is the same as the singular, and is only found on certain verbs).

In most descriptions of the language the active and attainative voices have been mistakenly termed transitive and intransitive respectively. Ttransitive, intransitive, passive, antipassive and 'antipassive passive' in the language are syntactic categories, and are formed by the interplay of nominal and verbal morphology, clause/sentence-level characteristics such as word-order, and semantic considerations.

Verb morphology consists of prefixes (aspect, positioning, etc.), suffixes (voice, number, and two fossilised multiplicative/causative suffixes) and endings (tense, aspect and mood). The structural matrix of a verb is :

(prefix)+(prefix)+stem(+FOSSILISED SUFFIX)+(VOICE)+(number)+ending(+ending).

Example:

pabalkabuthamadhin two were laid down across something [which would clear in the context]

pabalkabuthemadhin two lay down (laid themselves down) across something [which would clear in the context]'

prefix: pa- completive

prefix: bal- positional - across

stem: kabutha- place, lay

voice suffix: -Ø attainative, -i active

number suffix: -ma dual

tense-aspect ending: -dhin remote past perfective

Sample verb declension : íma- see, observe, supervise, examine, try, test

Tensed forms
Case/Suffix Perfective Attainative Imperfective Attainative Perfective Active Imperfective Active
remote future singular imane imaipu (imaiparui) imedhe imepu (imeparui)
dual imamane imampu (imamparui) imemadhe imempu (imemparui)
plural imamœine imamœipu (imamœiparui) imemœidhe imemœipu (imemœiparui)
near future singular imaipa imaipu (imaiparui) imepa imepu (imeparui)
dual imampa imampu (imamparui) imempa imempu (imemparui)
plural imamœipa imamœipu (imamœiparui) imemœipa imemœipu (imemœiparui)
present singular iman imaipa imiz imepa
dual imaman imampa imeman imempa
plural imamœin imamœipa imemœin imemœipa
today past singular imanu imadha imema imedha
dual imamanu imamadha imemanu imemadha
plural imamœinu imamœidha imemœinu imemœidha
recent past singular imangu imarngu imaingu imairngu
dual imamangu imamarngu imemangu imemarngu
plural imamœingu imamœirngu imemœingu imemœirngu
recent past singular imadhin imar imaidhin imai
dual imamadhin imamar imemadhin imemar
plural imamœidhin imamœi (imamir) imemœidhin imemœi (imemir)


Non-tensed forms
Case/Suffix Singular Dual Plural
Attainative Habitual imaipu (imaiparui) imampu (imamparui) imamœipu (imamœiparui)
Active Habitual imepu (imeparui) imempu (imemparui) imemœipu (imemœiparui)
Perfective Attainative Imperative (Singular Subject) imar imamar imamœi (imamir)
(Non-Plural Subject) imau (imaziu) imamariu imamœi (imamœiziu, imamiu)
Active Imperative imi imemariu imemœi (imemœiziu, imemiu)
Imperfective Attainative Imperative imadha imamadha imamœidha
Imperfective Active Imperative imedha imemadha imemœidha


Nominalised forms
Case/Suffix Verbal Noun Have-Form Privative Resultative
base form imai imail imaigi imaizi
independent impersonal form (nom-acc) imai imailnga imaiginga imaizinga
stem imai- imailmai- imaigimai- imaizimai-
independent personal form (nom-acc) ---- imailaig imaigig imaizig
stem ---- imailga- imaigiga- imaiziga-

Miscellaneous Paradigms

Three paradigms that have irregular morphology are:

Si[ ]kai Perhaps/Maybe/Possibly
(all dialects except Kalau Kawau Ya - which has invariable sike, sikedh)

masculine sinukai/senukai, feminine sinakai/senakai, general (singular, dual, plural) sikai

Goodbye

Singular yawa, Non-singular yawal

Hey! (word used to attract someone's attention)

mas. kame, kamedh, fem. kake, kakedh, non-singular kole, koledh

In sikedh, kamedh, kakedh and koledh, the -dh final is only found in more emphatic use.
The source of this article is wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.  The text of this article is licensed under the GFDL.
 
x
OK